The next time you find yourself in a heated argument, absolutely certain of your position, consider this: researchers have discovered that the more confident you feel about your stance, the more likely you are to be working with incomplete information. It’s a psychological quirk that might explain everything from family disagreements to international conflicts.
We’ve all been there: stuck in traffic, grumbling about the “idiot” driving too slowly in front of us or the “maniac” who just zoomed past. But what if that slow driver is carefully transporting a wedding cake, or the speeding car is rushing someone to the hospital? The fascinating new study published in PLOS ONE suggests that these snap judgments stem from what researchers call “the illusion of information adequacy” — our tendency to believe we have enough information to make sound decisions, even when we’re missing crucial details.
“We found that, in general, people don’t stop to think whether there might be more information that would help them make a more informed decision,” explains study co-author Angus Fletcher, a professor of English at The Ohio State University and member of the university’s Project Narrative, in a statement. “If you give people a few pieces of information that seems to line up, most will say ‘that sounds about right’ and go with that.”
In today’s polarized world, where debates rage over everything from vaccines to climate change, understanding why people maintain opposing viewpoints despite access to the same information has never been more critical. This research, conducted by Fletcher, Hunter Gehlbach of Johns Hopkins University, and Carly Robinson of Stanford University, reveals that we rarely pause to consider what information we might be missing before making judgments.
The researchers conducted an experiment with 1,261 American participants recruited through the online platform Prolific. The study centered around a hypothetical scenario about a school facing a critical decision: whether to merge with another school due to a drying aquifer threatening their water supply.
The participants were divided into three groups. One group received complete information about the situation, including arguments both for and against the merger. The other two groups only received partial information – either pro-merger or pro-separation arguments. The remarkable finding? Those who received partial information felt just as competent to make decisions as those who had the full picture.
“Those with only half the information were actually more confident in their decision to merge or remain separate than those who had the complete story,” Fletcher notes. “They were quite sure that their decision was the right one, even though they didn’t have all the information.”
Social media users might recognize this pattern in their own behavior: confidently sharing or commenting on articles after reading only headlines or snippets, feeling fully informed despite missing crucial context. It’s a bit like trying to review a movie after watching only the first half, yet feeling qualified to give it a definitive rating.
The study revealed an interesting finding regarding the influence of new information. When participants who initially received only one side of the story were later presented with opposing arguments, about 55% maintained their original position on the merger decision. That rate is comparable to that of the control group, which had received all information from the start.
Fletcher notes that this openness to new information might not apply to deeply entrenched ideological issues, where people may either distrust new information or try to reframe it to fit their existing beliefs. “But most interpersonal conflicts aren’t about ideology,” he points out. “They are just misunderstandings in the course of daily life.”
Beyond personal relationships, this finding has profound implications for how we navigate complex social and political issues. When people engage in debates about controversial topics, each side might feel fully informed while missing critical pieces of the puzzle. It’s like two people arguing about a painting while looking at it from different angles: each sees only their perspective but assumes they’re seeing the whole picture.
Fletcher, who studies how people are influenced by the power of stories, emphasizes the importance of seeking complete information before taking a stand. “Your first move when you disagree with someone should be to think, ‘Is there something that I’m missing that would help me see their perspective and understand their position better?’ That’s the way to fight this illusion of information adequacy.”
Source : https://studyfinds.org/science-confirms-know-it-alls-typically-know-less-than-they-think/